Archived posting to the Leica Users Group, 2004/02/06
[Author Prev] [Author Next] [Thread Prev] [Thread Next] [Author Index] [Topic Index] [Home] [Search]Thanks for the very impressive summary. I didn't know about the subtypes of M and L cones. I think you took care of the horse pretty well! I also believe that subcortical and cortical structures (programmed by a combination of genetic inheritance and individual experience/environment) normally have a greater influence on human perception of color than does the retina. (Leaving out such situations as a complete lack of a class of cone, of course.) The challenge for neurobiologists is to figure out where in the brain and (more interestingly) how that processing takes place. - -Aaron At 07:15 PM 2/5/2004, you wrote: >In a senior moment, I don't recall if I sent this post. If it a duplicate, I >apologize for the wasted bandwidth. > >Larry Z >--------------------------- > >To continue to beat a horse of a different color, this is a long post, >extracted from current research literature, explaining the assertion that >there are >more than three types of color receptors in the human eye. The trichromatic >theory of color vision is not invalidated, as such, but it has been >significantly modified. It helps explain why we can see colors that we >cannot normally >photograph and why people disagree on the exact color match of pieces of >fabric. >(This is my problem when my wife takes me shopping with her.) > >A long accepted fundamental property of human vision is trichromacy. The >trichromatic theory helps to explain our color perceptions and color >discriminations. The anatomical basis of trichromacy begins with the >complement of cone >photoreceptors in the retina. For over one hundred years researchers >thought that >the color-normal eye contained three cone types, designated as S, M, and L, >whose photopigments were later psychophysically estimated to have peak >spectral >sensitivities near 440, 540, and 560 nanometers. There is considerable >overlap in sensitivity of the middle wavelength sensitive and long wavelength >sensitive cone types. > >Over the years, however, sensory psychologists questioned whether subtle >variations may exist in normal color vision based on small individual >differences >in the spectral sensitivities of the photopigments (Alpern & Wake, 1977; >Neitz >& Jacobs, 1986). The findings of the early studies were viewed with some >skepticism, however, because of the difficulty in ruling out measurement >error and >confounding factors. As the psychophysical evidence grew, researchers began >to investigate this possibility from many angles. > >Today, psychophysical (Neitz & Jacobs, 1990; Mollon, 1992), >microspectrophotometric (Dartnall, Bowmaker, & Mollon, 1983), and >molecular genetic studies >(Nathans, Piantanida, Eddy, Shows, & Hogness, 1986; Winderickx et al., 1992) >provide evidence of substantial variation in the number and spectral >sensitivity >of the cone types in the color-normal eye (also see Mollon, Cavonius, and >Zrenner, 1998). The evidence now suggests the presence of three broad >families of >normally occurring cone photopigments. There is thought to be only one >photopigment with a peak spectral sensitivity in the short wavelengths >(blue), but >there is now evidence that there are multiple middle wavelength (green) >photopigments and multiple long wavelength (red) photopigments. The >difference in >spectral sensitivity among the middle wavelength pigments or among the long >wavelength pigments has been estimated to be approximately 5-7nm(Neitz, >Neitz, & >Jacobs, 1995). In fact there may be as many as 9 different cone types with >various >peaks in photosensitivity among the middle and long wavelength families. > >Molecular genetic analyses show that individuals may inherit a surprisingly >large number of different X-linked, recessive genes that encode the >production >of these photopigments (Neitz, Neitz, & Grishok, 1995). An obvious >question is >why do we have so many color vision genes? The genes that encode the middle >and long wavelength sensitive pigments reside near the end of one of the arms >of the X chromosome and they have very similar DNA sequences. In fact, the >substitution of one amino acid in the DNA of a photopigment gene is >sufficient to >cause a change in the spectral sensitivity of that photopigment and in our >color perceptions. The location and similarity of these genes makes them >susceptible to the kinds of genetic errors that produce multiple gene >copies, as well >as hybrid genes that are genetic composites of the original ones (Nathans, et >al., 1986). > >At present, it appears that normal color vision results from inheriting at >least one cone type from each cone class (short, middle, and long). It is >unclear, however, which complement of genes and cone types result in >specific types >of color vision deficiency. There is a great deal of genetic variation among >individuals with the same type of color defect, making this work difficult. >However, it appears that both the type and severity of a color vision >defect can >be linked to the complement of different cone types in the retina. Hybrid >genes, which have been associated with small differences in the spectral >sensitivity of the photopigments, are thought to be involved. > >These findings lead to an interesting question: if humans possess more than >three cone types in their retina, do they still have trichromatic vision? The >answer appears to be yes, presumably because the outputs of the different >middle or longwavelength cone photoreceptors are summed together before >leaving the >retina. The resulting signals differ to a small but significant degree across >individuals, though, because they affect color perception in some situations. >Individuals with different complements of cone pigments will not accept each >other's color matches in the long wavelength end of the spectrum and they >will >disagree on color names for certain wavelengths of light (Neitz, Neitz, & >Jacobs, 1993). For example, a particular mixture of red and green light might >appear a perfect yellow to your eye, but appear a greenish-yellow or slightly >orange to someone else. This type of color vision assessment, called the >Rayleigh >Match, is the most accurate method for measuring color discrimination and >diagnosing the congenital color vision defects. > >The distribution of photoreceptors in the retina appears to be nearly random. >The ratio of R / G / B cone types varies, but the long wavelength cones are >the most prevalent; short wavelength cones the least prevalent in the retina. >Thus some people with a normal complement of color vision genes, may have a >mosaic retina: a patchwork of color-normal and color-deficient regions (Cohn, >Emmerich, & Carlson, 1989). The nature of this mosaic depends on the >inherited >complement of color vision genes and on the point in development that >X-chromosome inactivation occurred. That is, some may develop a color vision >deficiency while others may develop normal color vision (Miyahara, >Pokorny, Smith, >Baron, & Baron, 1998). And, in fact, there are reports in the literature of >identical (monozygotic) twins where one twin has normal color vision and >the second >is color-deficient (Jorgenson, et al., 1992). > >In addition to teasing out the exact mechanism of color vision, work for >sensory psychologists and physiologists also involves investigating the >extent to >which these individual differences in color vision affect interactions with >the world. Society uses color to code information in a variety of settings, >including art. photography, education and transportation. In many occupations >color discrimination is critical, for example, in discriminating >electrical wiring >and colored signal lights or in medical research. While these individual >differences are small, they may prove to be problematic in some settings. > >In contrast to the research directed at the earliest stages of sensory >processing, today there is also substantial exciting research interest at >the other >end of the sensation to perception continuum: This research is directed at >higher level perceptual processes and phenomena in the gray area where >perception >and cognition meld. Culture, desire, expectation, and learning are as >important in determining what we see as the sensation itself. I suspect >that this >area is of greater interest to photographers than understanding the specific >mechanism of color vision. In short, each of us sees color slightly >differently >and there are some colors that humans can see which cannot be duplicated >by any >trichromatic process using fixed primary colors. > >Larry Z >-- >To unsubscribe, see http://mejac.palo-alto.ca.us/leica-users/unsub.html - -- To unsubscribe, see http://mejac.palo-alto.ca.us/leica-users/unsub.html